Language and Cognition
Language
is an important mechanism used in most individual’s everyday life. It helps
define their culture, their backgrounds, who they are and where their place in
the world is. Most of us don’t put a lot of thought into what mental processes
work together to allow an individual to express his thoughts and ideas through
language, but it is impossible to analyze the language development process
without factoring in the important role that cognition plays in that event.
This essay intends to present different hypothesis that try to explain how
cognition and language are related, and how deeply connected they must be in
order to allow the proper functioning and interaction of individuals in
society. As thinkers try to understand if people who speak different languages
think differently, or if there is a certain level of cognitive development
required to allow language skills acquisition, the debate concerned with the
relationship between language and cognition is one that will see no end in the
near future.
Language
can be defined as the human capacity for acquiring and using complex systems of
communication. This system allows individuals to express and communicate
thoughts and feelings by using speech sounds and written symbols. The
scientific study of language is linguistics. There are four key features of
language, which are phonemes, words, sentences and text. Phonemes-phonetics can
be considered the most important key feature of languages, because this aspect
is what makes each language different from one another. Phonemes are sounds
usually indicated by slash symbols, and these sounds cannot be broken into
smaller sounds. Phonetics studies the physical properties of those speech
sounds, and what they mean. Words, the second key feature in languages can be defined
as a unit of language formed by one of more spoken sounds. When words are put
together, sentences are formed. Sentences are grammatical units of one word or
more. Text refers to conversations, or a group of sentences put together to relay
a message. The four levels of language structure and processing are phonemes,
words, sentences, and texts. Phonemes make up words, which make up sentences,
which make up texts.
Is
language fundamentally different than other cognitive abilities, or does it
involve the same mental processes and characteristics? It is still unknown to
scientists how exactly language and cognition interact during the thinking
process, or how fundamental language is for thinking. “Language accumulates
cultural wisdom; cognition develops mental representations modeling surrounding
world and adapts cultural knowledge to concrete circumstances of life. Language
is acquired from surrounding language “ready-made” and therefore can be acquired
early in life. This early acquisition of language in childhood encompasses the
entire hierarchy from sounds to words, to phrases, and to highest concepts
existing in culture. Cognition is developed from experience. Yet cognition
cannot be acquired from experience alone; language is a necessary intermediary,
a ‘teacher.’” (Perlovsky, 2011).
Although
language development is a process that most individuals take for granted, as
many other cognitive functions, it must be learned. Children, for instance, not
only have to learn the names for thousands of objects, feelings, situations,
colors, animals, and more, but they also have to learn how to put those words
together and form cohesive sentences. Chomsky developed in the 1950s an
interesting concept of how and which mind mechanisms influenced the language
process. “It seemed obvious to Chomsky that surrounding language cultures do
not carry enough information for a child to learn language, unless specific
language learning mechanisms are inborn. These mechanisms should be specific
enough for learning complex language grammars and still flexible enough so that
a child of any ethnicity from any part of the world would learn whichever
language is spoken around.” (Perlovsky, 2011). Chomsky’s biggest innovation was
to interpret language abilities as akin to a mental organ. He believed that
children were born with a “language acquisition device” and some linguistic
knowledge. He also believed that children did not learn how to speak solely by
imitation adults, as they will often use incorrect verbal forms that they have
not learned from adults, like “goed” or speaked”. These kinds of mistakes,
according to Chomsky, happen because instead of just imitating adults, children
extract rules from what they constantly hear. “Because the language imput to
children is mfull of mistakes, stops and restarts, Chomsky felt that children
could not learn the language using general purpose problem-solving or
regularity-extraction skills” (Harris, n.d.). A lexicon, on the other hand, is
the vocabulary of a language, and what the sounds mean in that language. It is
impossible to know a language without knowing its lexicon, or its vocabulary.
Scientists
and psychologies have been trying for decades to understand to what extent is
human thinking dependent on the possession and understanding of a language. Is
it possible for humans to think without language skills? Some philosophers
believe that it is conceptually necessary that all thoughts are dependent on
language skills. On the other hand, there are philosophers that believe that
thoughts are independent of language, not only conceptually but also
metaphysically and causally (Carruthers, 2012). In between these two very
different ideas, there are many others that believe that some language is
necessary in the thinking processes, if not always, at least at times.
During
the second quarter of the twentieth century, behaviorism was the predominant
school of thought in psychology. Psychologists believed that most kinds on
animal behavior were associated to the response to a stimulus. At that time,
Benjamin Lee Whorf, an American linguist, presented the idea that because of
linguistic differences in grammar and usage, speakers of different languages
conceptualize and experience the world differently. He was impressed at the
variety of grammatical forms and modes of conceptualization present in the many
languages throughout the world. For instance, some languages do not have
specific words for what we know as “left” or “right”, but use instead
geocentric coordinates like east, west, north or south. He also though it was
interesting how some cultures on known colors as light or dark, instead of having
a specific name for each one. Eskimos, on the other hand, have countless words
just to describe the snow. Whorf proposed than that all these cultural
linguistic differences impact the cognitive processes of people in each of those
cultures. He also proposed that a deep relation exists between language and
thought. His theory, which was known as the Whorfian hypothesis (or the
Sapir-Whorf hypothesis), in its strongest formulation this hypothesis has it
that the structure of one’s native language determines one’s conceptual
categorization of the world. The Whorfian hypothesis generated a good deal of
debate and empirical testing in the 1950s and 1960s. (Carruthers, 2012).
Although Whorf’s ideas are still popular in some areas of social sciences, the
cognitive revolution that occurred in psychology in the early years of his
period made that some of his main ideas would fall into dispute amongst
cognitive scientists, especially during the second half of the twentieth
century.
The
first half of the twentieth century presented an important question, as
scientists, philosophers and psychologists tried to understand if the
relationship between cognition and language was influenced by each language’s
grammatical structures and vocabulary. With the upcoming arrival of cognitive
psychology, a new question would arise: are cognition and language similar or
different human abilities? This question has received many controversial
answers for the past few decades. Nowadays, scientists emphasize that our
species have evolved thanks to the distinction between different areas of human
ability, either some natural to individuals, such as language and basic number
use, and areas that require learning, like algebra or playing a musical
instrument.
References
Carruthers,
P. (2012). Language in cognition. Retrieved from http://faculty.philosophy.umd.edu/pcarruthers/Language%20in%20cognition.pdf
Harris,
C. L. (n.d.). Language and cognition. Retrieved from
http://www.bu.edu/psych/charris/papers/Encyclopedia.pdf
Leonid
Perlovsky, “Language and Cognition Interaction Neural Mechanisms,”
Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience, vol. 2011, Article ID 454587, 13
pages, 2011. doi:10.1155/2011/454587
No comments:
Post a Comment